logo logo European Journal of Educational Research

EU-JER is is a, peer reviewed, online academic research journal.

Subscribe to

Receive Email Alerts

for special events, calls for papers, and professional development opportunities.

Subscribe

Publisher (HQ)

Eurasian Society of Educational Research
Eurasian Society of Educational Research
Christiaan Huygensstraat 44, Zipcode:7533XB, Enschede, THE NETHERLANDS
Eurasian Society of Educational Research
Headquarters
Christiaan Huygensstraat 44, Zipcode:7533XB, Enschede, THE NETHERLANDS
Research Article

Young Children’s Responses to Social-Conventional Transgressions in Japanese Preschool Settings

Ai Mizokawa , Motoyuki Nakaya , Asuka Nomura

This study investigated young children’s recognition of social rules and responses to social-conventional transgressions in Japanese preschool s.


  • Pub. date: July 15, 2024
  • Online Pub. date: February 09, 2024
  • Pages: 1019-1029
  • 290 Downloads
  • 836 Views
  • 0 Citations
T

This study investigated young children’s recognition of social rules and responses to social-conventional transgressions in Japanese preschool settings. One hundred twenty-six children aged three to six years old heard four hypothetical stories that describe typical social-conventional transgressions in Japanese preschools. They were asked about their feelings when they witnessed each transgression (emotional response), whether they thought it was better to follow social rules and act like everyone else (social convention judgment), their justification for the judgment, and their willingness to play with the transgressor (interaction judgment). Most participants in all age groups valued following social rules and judged that the transgressor should act like everyone else, but five-year-olds generated more group- and other-oriented justifications for the judgment. Although there were no significant age group differences in interaction judgment, further analysis showed that three-year-olds’ willingness to play with the transgressor was positively related to their positive emotion, whereas such relationships were not found in four- and five-year-olds. The results demonstrate that Japanese children’s responses to social-conventional transgressions become more social and complex throughout young childhood in the Japanese cultural context.

Keywords: Preschool, social-conventional transgressions, social development, young children.

cloud_download PDF
Cite
Article Metrics
Views
290
Download
836
Citations
Crossref
0

Scopus

References

Aoi, R. (2000). Youji no nakamairi bamen ni okeru kihan no kinou [The function of social norms on the children's social regulation in the play-group entry situation]. The Annual of Research on Early Childhood, 22, 45–52. https://doi.org/10.15027/17847

Banerjee, R., Bennett, M., & Luke, N. (2012). Children’s reasoning about self-presentation following rule violations: The role of self-focused attention. Child Development, 83(5), 1805–1821. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01813.x

Danovitch, J., & Bloom, P. (2009). Children's extension of disgust to physical and moral events. Emotion, 9(1), 107–112. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014113

Haerpfer, C., Inglehart, R., Moreno, A., Welzel, C., Kizilova, K., Diez-Medrano, J., Lagos, M., Norris, P., Ponarin, E., & Puranen, B. (Eds.). (2022). World values survey: Round seven-country-pooled datafile version 5.0. JD Systems Institute & WVSA Secretariat. https://doi.org/10.14281/18241.20

Hardecker, S., Schmidt, M. F., Roden, M., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Young children’s behavioral and emotional responses to different social norm violations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 364–379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.06.012

Hasegawa, M. (2014a). Shinnen no tayousei ni tsuite no kodomo no rikai: Soutaishugi, kanyousei, kokoro no riron kara no kentou [Children’s understanding about diversity of beliefs from the perspectives of relativism, tolerance, and theory of mind]. The Japanese Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(4), 345–355. https://doi.org/10.11201/jjdp.25.345

Hasegawa, M. (2014b). Tasha no tayousei heno kanyou: Jidou to seinen ni okeru shudan kara no haiijyo ni tsuite no handan [Tolerance for diversity: Judgments of children, adolescents, and young adults when accepting others into an in-group]. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 62(1), 13–23. https://doi.org/10.5926/jjep.62.13

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature, 466(7302), Article 29. https://doi.org/10.1038/466029a

Hughes, C. (2011). Social understanding and social lives: From toddlerhood through to the transition to school. Psychology Press.

Inglehart, R., Haerpfer, C., Moreno, A., Welzel, C., Kizilova, K., Diez-Medrano, J., Lagos, M., Norris, P., Ponarin, E., & Puranen, B. (2018). World values survey: Round onecountry-pooled datafile. JD Systems Institute & WVSA Secretariat. https://doi.org/10.14281/18241.3

Kagan, J. (2005). Human morality and temperament. In G. Carlo & C. P. Edwards (Eds.), Moral motivation through the life span (pp. 1–32). University of Nebraska Press.

Killen, M., Crystal, D. S., & Watanabe, H. (2002). Japanese and American children's evaluations of peer exclusion, tolerance of differences, and prescriptions for conformity. Child Development, 73(6), 1788–1802. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00506

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224.

Mizokawa, A. (2011). 4, 5 saiji ni okeru usonaki no koushakaiteki koudou wo hikidasu kinou no ninshiki [Young children's understanding of the interpersonal functions of apparent crying]. The Japanese Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22(1), 33–43. https://doi.org/10.11201/jjdp.22.33

Munakata, Y., Yanaoka, K., Doebel, S., Guild, R. M., Michaelson, L. E., & Saito, S. (2020). Group influences on children’s delay of gratification: Testing the roles of culture and personal connections. Collabra: Psychology, 6(1), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.265

Nucci, L. (1981). Conceptions of personal issues: A domain distinct from moral or societal concepts. Child Development, 52(1), 114–121. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129220

Nucci, L. P., & Turiel, E. (1978). Social interactions and the development of social concepts in preschool children. Child Development, 49(2), 400–407. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128704

Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2005). The definition and selection of key competencies: Executive summary. Retrieved November 30, 2023, from https://bit.ly/4882fYu

Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2017). Starting strong IV: Early childhood education and care data country note JAPAN. Retrieved March 23, 2023, from https://bit.ly/41jTo3R

Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2019). OECD future of education and skills 2030 conceptual learning framework concept note: Student agency for 2030. Retrieved March 23, 2023, from https://bit.ly/4ahDE5p

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512

Shuto, T., & Ninomiya, K. (2003). Kodomo no doutokuteki jiritsu no hattatsu [Development of children’s moral autonomy]. Kazama Shobou.

Smetana, J. G. (1981). Preschool children's conceptions of moral and social rules. Child Development, 52(4), 1333–1336. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129527

Smetana, J. G. (1985). Preschool children’s conceptions of transgressions: The effects of varying moral and conventional domain-related attributes. Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 18–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.1.18

Smetana, J. G., Ball, C. L., Jambon, M., & Yoo, H. N. (2018). Are young children’s preferences and evaluations of moral and conventional transgressors associated with domain distinctions in judgments? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 284–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.04.008

Smetana, J. G., & Yoo, H. N. (2022). Development and variations in moral and social-conventional judgments: A social domain theory approach. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 19–36). Routledge.

Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge University Press.

Turiel, E. (1989). Domain-specific social judgments and domain ambiguities. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35(1), 89–114. https://www.jstor.org/stable/23086426

Turiel, E. (2006). The development of morality. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (Vol. 3, 6th ed., pp. 789–857). Wiley.

Van de Vondervoort, J. W., & Hamlin, J. K. (2017). Preschoolers’ social and moral judgments of third-party helpers and hinderers align with infants’ social evaluations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 164, 136–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.07.004

Watanabe, N., Denham, S. A., Jones, N. M., Kobayashi, T., Bassett, H. H., & Ferrier, D. E. (2019). Working toward cross-cultural adaptation: Preliminary psychometric evaluation of the affect knowledge test in Japanese preschoolers. SAGE Open, 9(2), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244019846688

Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00304

Yoshida, M. (2011). Youji ni okeru mirai no jiko no joutai ni tsuite no yosoku: Mirai no fukakujitsusei heno kiduki to shinpai [The ability of preschoolers to predict the future: Awareness and concern regarding the uncertainty of the future]. Japanese Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22(1), 44–54. https://doi.org/10.11201/jjdp.22.44

Yucel, M., Hepach, R., & Vaish, A. (2020). Young children and adults show differential arousal to moral and conventional transgressions. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 548. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00548

...